Document Type : Research Paper I Open Access I Released under CC BY-NC 4.0 license

Authors

1 Instructor, Department of Motor Behavior, Faculty of Sport Sciences, Shahid Chamran University of Ahvaz, Ahvaz, Iran

2 MSc of Motor Behavior, Faculty of Sport Sciences, Shahid Chamran University of Ahvaz, Ahvaz, Iran

3 Assistant Professor, Department of Motor Behavior, Faculty of Sport Sciences, Shahid Chamran University of Ahvaz, Ahvaz, Iran

Abstract

The aim of this study was to investigate the effect of handedness and task difficulty on effective target width and temporal accuracy of the Fitts’ speed-accuracy tradeoff task. The present study was semi-experimental and the tools used included Edinburgh handedness questionnaire, light pen, speed-accuracy measurement device, laptop, chronometer, and metronome. The statistical population consisted of students aged between 14 and 15. The sample included 20 students who participated in this study by convenience sampling method. They were divided into two groups: left-hand and right-hand (each group 10 subjects). Each participant performed 4 trials (each trial 30 seconds) of dual target tapping task in rhythm with the metronome sound. The trials consisted of two easy and difficult tasks and each subject performed each task with dominant and not-dominant hands in rhythm with the metronome sound set up at 300 milliseconds. For statistical analysis of data, variance analysis with repeated measures was used at the significance level of 0.05. The results showed that in dominant hand, the handedness and difficulty of the task had no significant effect on the effective width of the target (P=0.973, P=0.611). Also, the handedness did not affect the average time lag (P=0.135, P=0.785), but in non-dominant hand, the average time lag was more for the difficult task than the simple task (P=0.001). In difficult tasks, participants seem to be trying to keep the spatial error rate constant by reducing the speed of the movement and to sacrifice speed for the spatial accuracy. Also, the time error (mean lag) is more influenced by the difficulty of the task than the handedness.

Keywords

1. Schmidt R A LTD. Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis. 4, editor: Human Kinetics; 2005.
2. Van Veen V, Krug MK, Carter CS. The neural and computational basis of controlled speed-accuracy tradeoff during task performance. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2008;20(11):1952-65.
3. Rozand V, Lebon F, Papaxanthis C, Lepers R. Effect of mental fatigue on speed–accuracy trade-off. Neuroscience. 2015;297:219-30.
4. Fitts PM. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of experimental psychology. 1954;47(6):381.
5. Elliott D, Hansen S, Grierson LE, Lyons J, Bennett SJ, Hayes SJ. Goal-directed aiming: two components but multiple processes. Psychological bulletin. 2010;136(6):1023.
6. Ifft P, Lebedev M, Nicolelis MA. Cortical correlates of Fitts’ law. Frontiers in integrative neuroscience. 2011;5:85.
7. Plamondon R, Alimi AM. Speed/accuracy trade-offs in target-directed movements. Behavioral and brain sciences. 1997;20(2):279-303.
8. Danion F, Bongers RM, Bootsma RJ. The trade-off between spatial and temporal variabilities in reciprocal upper-limb aiming movements of different durations. PloS one. 2014;9(5):e97447.
9. Peternel L, Sigaud O, Babič J. Unifying speed-accuracy trade-off and cost-benefit trade-off in human reaching movements. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2017;11:615.
10. Shadmehr R, De Xivry JJO, Xu-Wilson M, Shih T-Y. Temporal discounting of reward and the cost of time in motor control. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30(31):10507-16.
11. Rigoux L, Guigon E. A model of reward-and effort-based optimal decision making and motor control. PLoS computational biology. 2012;8(10):e1002716.
12. Young WB, Bilby GE. The effect of voluntary effort to influence speed of contraction on strength, muscular power, and hypertrophy development. The Journal of Strength & Conditioning Research. 1993;7(3):172-8.
13. Green L, Myerson J. A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychological bulletin. 2004;130(5):769.
14. Grouios G. Right hand advantage in visually guided reaching and aiming movements: brief review and comments. Ergonomics. 2006;49(10):1013-7.
15. Bagi J, Kudachi, P., & Goudar, S. . Influence of motor task on handedness. Al Ameen Journal of Medical Sciences. 2011;4(1):87-91.
16. Asai T, Sugimori E, Tanno Y. Two agents in the brain: motor control of unimanual and bimanual reaching movements. PloS one. 2010;5(4):e10086.
17. Moghadam A NNM, Rezaeian F. Comparation of the effect of ipsilateral and contralateral eye-hand dominant on the accuracy of the free throwing of basketball players. Quarterly Journal of Sport Sciences. 2002;2(8):35-44.
18. Bisiacchi PS, Ripoll H, Stein JF, Simonet P, Azemar G. Left-handedness in fencers: An attentional advantage? Perceptual and motor skills. 1985;61(2):507-13.
19. Gursoy R. Effects of left-or right-hand preference on the success of boxers in Turkey. British Journal of Sports Medicine. 2009;43(2):142-4.
20. Holtzen DW. Handedness and professional tennis. International Journal of neuroscience. 2000;105(1-4):101-19.
21. Mickevičienė D, Motiejūnaitė K, Karanauskienė D, Skurvydas A, Vizbaraitė D, Krutulytė G, et al. Gender-dependent bimanual task performance. Medicina. 2011;47(9):73.
22. Parish A, Dwelly P, Baghurst T, Lirgg C. Effect of handedness on gross motor skill acquisition in a novel sports skill task. Perceptual and motor skills. 2013;117(2):449-56.
23. Zuoza A, Skurvydas A, Mickeviciene D, Gutnik B, Zouzene D, Penchev B, et al. Behavior of dominant and non dominant arms during ballistic protractive target-directed movements. Human physiology. 2009;35(5):576-84.
24. Kabbash P, MacKenzie IS, Buxton W, editors. Human performance using computer input devices in the preferred and non-preferred hands. Proceedings of the INTERACT'93 and CHI'93 Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems; 1993: ACM.
25. Stöckel T, Weigelt M. Brain lateralisation and motor learning: Selective effects of dominant and non-dominant hand practice on the early acquisition of throwing skills. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition. 2012;17(1):18-37.
26. Lenhard A, Hoffmann J. Constant error in aiming movements without visual feedback is higher in the preferred hand. Laterality. 2007;12(3):227-38.
27. Harris LJ. In fencing, what gives left-handers the edge? Views from the present and the distant past. Laterality. 2010;15(1-2):15-55.
28. Goldstein SR, Young CA. " Evolutionary" stable strategy of handedness in major league baseball. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 1996;110(2):164.
29. Grondin S, Guiard Y, Ivry RB, Koren S. Manual laterality and hitting performance in major league baseball. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1999;25(3):747.
30. Brooks R, Bussiere LF, Jennions MD, Hunt J. Sinister strategies succeed at the cricket World Cup. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences. 2004;271(suppl_3):S64-S6.
31. Grouios G. Motoric dominance and sporting excellence: Training versus heredity. Perceptual and motor skills. 2004;98(1):53-66.
32. Latash ML. Neurophysiological basis of movement: Human Kinetics; 2008.
33. Taheri H R TR, Kheyrandish A. . the effect of manipulated of the distance and width of target on learning in basketball free threw: according to index of difficulty. Motor Behavior. 2014;19(1):15-30. (in persian)
34. Gutnik B, Skurvydas A, Zuoza A, Zuoziene I, Mickevičienė D, Alekrinskis B, et al. Influence of spatial accuracy constraints on reaction time and maximum speed of performance of unilateral movements. Perceptual and motor skills. 2015;120(2):519-33.
35. Hancock PA, Newell KM. The movement speed-accuracy relationship in space-time. Motor Behavior: Springer; 1985. p. 153-88.
36. Schmidt RA, Zelaznik H, Hawkins B, Frank JS, Quinn Jr JT. Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. Psychological review. 1979;86(5):415.
37. Harris CM, Wolpert DM. Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature. 1998;394(6695):780.
38. Todorov E, Jordan MI. Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nature neuroscience. 2002;5(11):1226.
39. Selen LP, Beek PJ, Van Dieën JH. Impedance is modulated to meet accuracy demands during goal-directed arm movements. Experimental Brain Research. 2006;172(1):129-38.