نوع مقاله : مقاله پژوهشی Released under CC BY-NC 4.0 license I Open Access I
نویسندگان
1 گروه تربیت بدنی و علوم ورزشی، دانشگاه پیام نور، ایران.
2 گروه رفتار حرکتی و روانشناسی ورزشی، دانشکدة علوم ورزشی و تندرستی، دانشگاه تهران، تهران، ایران.
3 گروه رفتار حرکتی و روانشناسی ورزشی، دانشکدة علوم ورزشی و تندرستی، دانشگاه تهران، تهران، ایران .
4 گروه رفتار حرکتی و روانشناسی ورزشی ، دانشکدة علوم ورزشی و تندرستی، دانشگاه تهران، تهران، ایران.
چکیده
مقدمه: هدف از پژوهش حاضر بررسی تأثیر تمرین بدنی و الگوی مشاهدهای انتخابی و اجباری بر میزان انگیزۀ درونی و یادگیری شناختی مهارت شوت جفت بسکتبال با رویکرد نیازهای روانشناختی پایه بود.
روش پژوهش: 80 دانشجوی پسر (۱۸-22 سال) بهصورت داوطلبانه انتخاب و براساس نمرات پیشآزمون بهصورت تصادفی در چهار گروه (20 نفر) خودکنترل، جفتشده، آزمونگر کنترل و کنترل قرار گرفتند. برای بررسی میزان انگیزه، از پرسشنامۀ انگیزۀ درونی ((IMI وبرای بررسی میزان یادگیری شناختی از پرسشنامۀ فراخوانی (اقتباس از نودسون، 1993) استفاده شد. ویدئوی آموزشی از مدل ماهر اجزای حرکت را نمایش داد و به آزمودنیها ارائه شد. آزمون یادداری24ساعت بعد انجام گرفت. برای بررسی نتایج از تحلیل واریانس یکراهه (آنوا) استفاده شد (0/05≥P).
یافتهها: نتایج نشان داد در مرحلۀ اکتساب میزان انگیزۀ درونی برای یادگیری مهارت شوت جفت بسکتبال آزمودنیها در شرایط مشاهدۀ خودکنترل بهطور معناداری بالاتر از آزمودنیها در شرایط جفتشده و کنترل بود. همچنین در مرحلۀ یادداری میزان انگیزۀ درونی در شرایط مشاهده خودکنترل بهطور معناداری بالاتر از دیگر شرایط آزمایشی بود. علاوهبر این در مراحل اکتساب و یادداری یادگیری شناختی (موفقیت فراخوانی) آزمودنیها در شرایط مشاهدۀ خودکنترل بهطور معناداری بالاتر از آزمودنیهای در شرایط جفتشده و کنترل بود.
نتیجهگیری: بهطور کلی نتایج این پژوهش نشان داد که انگیزه بر یادگیری تأثیر دارد، همچنین نمایش تکلیف بهصورت کل- بخش- کل روشی مؤثر و کارامدی در مرحلۀ شناختی یادگیری مهارت است.
کلیدواژهها
عنوان مقاله [English]
The Effect of Physical Practice and Selective and Compulsive 0bservational Pattern on Inner Motivation and Cognitive Learning of Basketball Jump Shot: with regard to the basic Psychological Needs
نویسندگان [English]
- Farzad Maleki 1
- Mahmood Sheikh 2
- Fazlallah Bagherzadeh 3
- Rasoul Hemayattalab 4
1 Department of Physical Education, University of Payam Noor, Iran.
2 Department of Motor Behavior and Sports Psychology, Faculty of Sport Sciences and Health, University of Tehran, Tehran, Iran.
3 Department of Motor Behavior and Sports Psychology , Faculty of Sport Sciences and Health, University of Tehran, Tehran, Iran .
4 Department of Motor Behavior and Sports Psychology, Faculty of Sport Sciences and Health, University of Tehran, Tehran , Iran.
چکیده [English]
Introduction: This research aimed to investigate the effect of physical training and selective and compulsory observational pattern on the intrinsic motivation and cognitive learning of basketball jump shot skill with the approach of basic psychological demands.
Methods: Eighty male students (18-22 years old) were selected voluntarily and based on their pre-test scores, were randomly divided into four Self-Control, Paired, Experimenter-Control, and Control groups (20 people in each group). To examine the motivation level, the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI), and to examine the cognitive learning level, Recall Questionnaire (adapted from Knudson, 1993) was used. An educational video of the expert model that showed the movement components was presented to the participants. A retention test was conducted 24 hours later. One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to examine the data (p≥0.05).
Results: The results showed that in the acquisition phase, the level of intrinsic motivation of the participants to learn the basketball jump shot skill in the Self-Control observation condition was significantly higher than the participants in the Paired and Control conditions. Also, in the retention phase, the level of intrinsic motivation in the Self-Control observation condition was significantly higher than in other experimental conditions. In addition, in the acquisition and retention phases of cognitive learning (recall success), the participants in the Self-Control observation condition scored significantly higher than the participants in the Paired and Control conditions.
Conclusion: In general, the results of this study show that motivation affects learning. Also, task demonstration as a whole-part-whole is an effective and efficient method in the cognitive phase.
کلیدواژهها [English]
- Demonstration
- Model
- Motivation
- Physical Practice
- Recall
Abdoli, B., Farsi, A., & Shojae, O. (2015). The effect of self-controlled observational practice and model's
skill level on badminton long serve. Motor Behavior, 7(20), 35-48 .(In Persian)
Al-Abood, S. A., Davids, K., & Bennett, S. J. (2001). Specificity of task constraints and effects of visual demonstrations and verbal instructions in directing learners' search during skill acquisition. Journal of Motor Behavior, 33(3), 295-305.
Bandura, A. (1966). The role of vicarious learning in personality development. Paper presented at the
18Mezhdunarodnyj psikhologicheskij kongress «Formirovanie lichnosti v kollektive. (pp. 84-92).
Bandura, A. (1969). Social-learning theory of identificatory processes. Handbook of socialization theory and
research, 213-262 .
Bandura, A. (1975). Analysis of modeling processes. School Psychology Review, 4(1), 4-10.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory: Englewood Cliffs,
NJ, US: Prentice-Hall, Inc. 23-2.
Bandura, A. (1993). Perceived self-efficacy in cognitive development and functioning. Educational psychologist, 28(2), 117-148.
Bandura A, J. F. (1991). Self-regulatory mechanisms governing the impact of social comparison on complex
decision making. Journal of personality and social psychology, 60, 941-951
Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. (1977). Social learning theory.
Baudry, L., Leroy, D., & Chollet, D. (2006). The effect of combined self-and expert-modelling on the
performance of the double leg circle on the pommel horse. Journal of sports sciences, 24(10),1055-1063 .
BG, S.-Z. (1992). Investigation the information nature of a modeled visual demonstration. (Ph.D), Louisiana State University.
Bird ,G., & Heyes, C. (2005). Effector-dependent learning by observation of a finger movement sequence.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(2), 262.
Blandin, Y., & Proteau, L. (2000). On the cognitive basis of observational learning: Development of
mechanisms for the detection and correction of errors. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Section A, 53(3), 846-867.
Bordenave, D. H. (2015). Examining the Learning Effects of Segmented Model Demonstrations on the Motor
& Cognitive Learning of the Basketball Jump Shot .
Buchanan, J. J., & Dean, N. J. (2010). Specificity in practice benefits learning in novice models and variability
in demonstration benefits observational practice. Psychological Research PRPF, 74(3), 326-313
Carroll, W. R., & Bandura, A. (1982). The role of visual monitoring in observational learning of action
patterns: Making the unobservable observable. Journal of Motor Behavior, 14(2), 153-167.
Carroll, W. R., & Bandura, A. (1985). Role of timing of visual monitoring and motor rehearsal in observational learning of action patterns. Journal of Motor Behavior, 17(3), 269-281.
Carroll, W. R., & Bandura, A. (1990). Representational guidance of action production in observational learning: A causal analysis. Journal of Motor Behavior, 22(1),97-85.
Chen, D., & Singer, R. N. (1992). Self-regulation and cognitive strategies in sport participation. International
Journal of Sport Psychology .
Chiviacowsky, S., & Wulf, G. (2002). Self-controlled feedback: Does it enhance learning because performers get feedback when they need it? Research quarterly for exercise and sport, 73(4),408-415.
Chiviacowsky, S., & Wulf, G. (2005). Self-controlled feedback is effective if it is based on the learner's performance. Research quarterly for exercise and sport,76(1),42-48.
Deci, E., & Ryan, R. (1990). I.(1990)./\motivational approach to self: Integration in personality. Z\] ebrr۱.
r/ea. Y)'I71j70. l ‘ltllfl on, 11(10),237-288 .
Deci, E., & Ryan, R. (2000). What is the self in self-directed learning? Findings from recent motivational
research. Conceptions of self-directed learning: Theoretical and conceptual considerations, 75-92 .
Downey, M. J. (1988). Effects of age, pre-task cues, and task complexity on response acquisition in observational learning: McGill University.
Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2004). How positive affect modulates cognitive control: reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30(2), 343.
Fagundes, J ,.Chen, D. D., & Laguna, P. (2013). Self-control and frequency of model presentation: Effects on
learning a ballet passé relevé. Human Movement Science, 32(4), 847-856.
Ferrari, M. (1996). Observing the observer: Self-regulation in the observational learning of motor skills. Developmental review, 16(2),203-240.
Friedrich, H. F., & Mandl, H. (1995). Analyse und Förderung selbstgesteuerten lernens: Dt. Inst für
Fernstudienforschung, Abt. Angewandte Kognitionswiss.
Ghobady N, D. A., Shojaei M. (2013). Comparing the Effects of self-modeling and expert models observation on performance and learning footsal side foot pass. European journal of experimental biology, 3(1),508-512.
Hodges, N. J., Chua, R., & Franks, I. M. (2003). The role of video in facilitating perception and action of a novel coordination movement. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35(3), 247-260.
Hutchinson, J. C., Sherman, T., Martinovic, N., & Tenenbaum, G. (2008). The effect of manipulated self-efficacy on perceived and sustained effort. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 20(4), 457-472.
Ilies, R., & Judge, T. A. (2005). Goal regulation across time: the effects of feedback and affect. Journal of applied psychology, 90(3), 453.
Izadpanah. (2014). Comparison the effects of self-control and Instructor- Control feedback on performance
and learning badminton services with regard to the basic psychological needs. (MSc), University of
Semnan. (In Persian)
Janelle, C. M., Barba, D. A., Frehlich, S. G., Tennant, L. K., & Cauraugh, J. H. (1997). Maximizing performance feedback effectiveness through videotape replay and a self-controlled learning environment. Research quarterly for exercise and sport, 68(4), 269-279.
Janelle, C. M., Kim, J., & Singer, R. N. (1995). Subject-controlled performance feedback and learning of a closed motor skill. Perceptual and motor skills, 81(2), 627-634.
Johnson, D. S., Turban, D. B., Pieper, K. F., & Ng, Y. M. (1996). Exploring the Role of Normative‐and Performance‐Based Feedback in Motivational Processes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26(11), 973.
Kampiotis, S., & Theodorakou, K. (2006). The influence of five different types of observation based teaching on the cognitive level of learning. Kinesiology, 38(2), 116-125.
Laguna, P. (1999). Effects of multiple correct model demonstrations on cognitive representation development
and performance accuracy in motor skill acquisition. Journal of human movement studies, 37(2), 55-86.
Laguna, P. L. (2008). Task complexity and sources of task-related information during the observational learning process. Journal of sports sciences, 26(10), 1097-1113.
Lewthwaite, R., & Wulf, G .(2010). Social-comparative feedback affects motor skill learning. Quarterly
journal of experimental psychology, 63(4),738-749 .
M, B. (1996). Self-regulated learning at the junction of cognition and motivation. European psychologist, 1,100-112.
Magill ,R. A., & Anderson, D. I. (2007). Motor learning and control: Concepts and applications (Vol. 11):
McGraw-Hill New York.
Maleki, F., Nia, P., Zarghami, M., & Neisi, A. (2010). The comparison of different types of observational
training on motor learning of gymnastic handstand. Journal of Human Kinetics, 26,13-19. (In Persian).
Maleki F, S. P., Zarghami, M, Kousari S. (2010). The Comparison of Three Different Educational Methods through Observation on Acquisition and Retention of Motor skill. Motor behavior and sports psychology, 7,31-44. (In Persian)
Maleki F, S. P., Zarghami, M, Ostovan Z. (2012). The influence various types of observation based teaching on cognitive learning handstand skill. Research on sports management and motor behavior, 16(8),89-106. (In Persian)
McAuley, E., Duncan, T., & Tammen, V. V. (1989). Psychometric properties of the Intrinsic Motivation
Inventory in a competitive sport setting: A confirmatory factor analysis. Research quarterly for exercise
and sport, 60(1),48-58 .
Metcalfe, J., & Finn, B. (2008). Evidence that judgments of learning are causally related to study choice. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(1),174-179.
Nicklin, J. M., & Williams, K. J. (2011). Self-regulation of goals and performance: Effects of discrepancy
feedback, regulatory focus, and self-efficacy. Psychology, 2(03),187 .
Patterson, J. T., & Lee, T. D. (2010). Self-regulated frequency of augmented information in skill learning.
Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 64(1),33 .
Pollock, B. J., & Lee, T. D. (1992). Effects of the model's skill level on observational motor learning. Research quarterly for exercise and sport, 63(1),25-29.
Rohbanfard, H., & Proteau, L. (2011). Learning through observation: a combination of expert and novice models favors learning. Experimental Brain Research, 215(4-3),183-197.
Ross, D., Bird, A. M., Doody, S. G., & Zoeller, M. (1985). Effects of modeling and videotape feedback with knowledge of results on motor performance. Human Movement Science, 4(2),149-157.
Sanli, E. A., Patterson, J. T., Bray, S. R., & Lee, T. D. (2013). Understanding self-controlled motor learning protocols through the self-determination theory. Frontiers in psychology, 3,611.
Schmidt RA, L. T. (2005). Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis (4 th ed.): Human Kinetics
Europe
Scully, D., & Carnegie, E. (1998). Observational learning in motor skill acquisition: A look at demonstrations. The Irish Journal of Psychology, 19(4),472-485.
Scully, D., & Newell, K. (1985). Observational-learning and the acquisition of motor-skills-toward a visualperception
perspective. Journal of human movement studies, 11(4),169-186.
Sheffield, F. D. (1961). Theoretical considerations in the learning of complex sequential tasks from
demonstration and practice. Student response in programmed instruction,13-32.
Southard, D., & Higgins, T. (1987). Changing movement patterns: Effects of demonstration and practice. Research quarterly for exercise and sport, 58(1),77-80.
Su, Y.-L & ,.Reeve, J. (2011). A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of intervention programs designed to
support autonomy. Educational Psychology Review, 23(1),159-188.
Weeks, D. L. (1992). A comparison of modeling modalities in the observational learning of an externally paced skill. Research quarterly for exercise and sport, 63(4),373-380.
Wulf, G., Clauss, A., Shea, C. H., & Whitacre, C. A. (2001). Benefits of self-control in dyad practice. Research quarterly for exercise and sport, 72(3),299-303.
Wulf, G., & Mornell, A. (2008). Insights about practice from the perspective of motor learning: a review. Music Perform Res, 2,1-25.
Wulf, G., Raupach, M., & Pfeiffer, F. (2005). Self-controlled observational practice enhances learning.
Research quarterly for exercise and sport, 76(1),107-111.
Wulf, G., & Toole, T. (1999). Physical assistance devices in complex motor skill learning: Benefits of a self-controlled practice schedule. Research quarterly for exercise and sport, 70(3),265-272.